Categorie: Theories

  • Curiosity

    Curiosity is a complex and powerful emotional reaction that filmmakers often aim to elicit in their audiences. Various techniques and effects can create curiosity in film, engaging viewers in the story and keeping them invested in it. This essay discusses some of the effects that can create curiosity in film.

    One of the most effective ways to create curiosity in film is to use suspense. Suspense involves delaying the resolution of a particular situation, creating a sense of tension and anticipation in the audience. Alfred Hitchcock was a master of this technique, and his films such as “Psycho” and “Vertigo” are filled with moments of suspense that keep viewers on the edge of their seats (Deutelbaum & Poague, 2011). In “Psycho”, the shower scene is filled with suspense as the audience knows that the killer is in the bathroom, but Marion does not. The use of suspense in this scene creates a sense of curiosity in the audience as they wait to see what will happen next.

    Another technique that can create curiosity in film is to use mystery. Mystery involves presenting the audience with a puzzle or a question that needs to be solved. This can be achieved through the use of enigmatic characters, strange events, or unexplained phenomena. David Lynch’s “Mulholland Drive” is an example of a film that uses mystery to create curiosity. The film is filled with cryptic clues and unexplained events that keep viewers guessing as to what is really going on (Gibson, 2016). The use of mystery in this film creates a sense of curiosity in the audience as they try to unravel the secrets of the story.

    Ambiguity is another technique that can create curiosity in film. Ambiguity involves presenting the audience with a situation or a character that is not clearly defined. This can be achieved through the use of unclear motives, conflicting emotions, or contradictory actions. Christopher Nolan’s “Inception” is an example of a film that uses ambiguity to create curiosity. The film is filled with complex and layered characters, each with their own motivations and desires. The use of ambiguity in this film creates a sense of curiosity in the audience as they try to understand the true nature of the story (Nolan, 2010).

    The unexpected is another technique that can create curiosity in film. The unexpected involves presenting the audience with a surprise or a twist that they were not expecting. This can be achieved through the use of unexpected events, unexpected character actions, or unexpected plot twists. M. Night Shyamalan’s “The Sixth Sense” is an example of a film that uses the unexpected to create curiosity. The film has a twist ending that completely changes the audience’s perception of the story, creating a sense of curiosity in the audience as they try to figure out how they missed the clues (Ebert, 1999).

    In addition to these techniques, there are other factors that can create curiosity as an emotional reaction in film. The use of music is one such factor. Music can set the tone for a scene, create a sense of tension or anticipation, and add emotional depth to the story. John Williams’ theme music in “Jaws” creates a sense of dread and anticipation in the audience, building up to the appearance of the shark (Sider, Freeman, & Sider, 2013). The use of music in this film creates a sense of curiosity in the audience as they wait to see what will happen next.

    Visual effects are another factor that can create curiosity in film. Visual effects can be used to create a sense of awe, wonder, or excitement in the audience. In “Avatar”, James Cameron used visual effects to create the stunning world of Pandora, immersing the audience in a world unlike anything they had seen before (Prince, 2013).The 

    use of visual effects in this film creates a sense of curiosity in the audience as they explore this new and unfamiliar world.

    Finally, the use of pacing can also create curiosity in film. Pacing involves the speed and rhythm at which the story is told, and it can be used to create a sense of tension and anticipation in the audience. Steven Spielberg’s “Jurassic Park” is an example of a film that uses pacing to create curiosity. The film starts off slowly, introducing the characters and the setting, but as the story progresses, the pace quickens, building up to the climactic finale (Young, 2000). The use of pacing in this film creates a sense of curiosity in the audience as they wait to see how the story will unfold.

    In conclusion, there are many techniques and effects that can create curiosity as an emotional reaction in film. Suspense, mystery, ambiguity, the unexpected, music, visual effects, and pacing are just some of the ways that filmmakers can engage their audiences and keep them invested in the story. By understanding how these techniques and effects work, filmmakers can create films that are not only entertaining but also emotionally engaging and thought-provoking.

    References:

    Deutelbaum, M. & Poague, L. (2011). A Hitchcock reader. John Wiley & Sons.

    Ebert, R. (1999). The Sixth Sense. Roger Ebert. https://www.rogerebert.com/reviews/the-sixth-sense-1999

    Gibson, S. (2016). Mulholland Drive. Harvard Film Archive. https://harvardfilmarchive.org/calendar/mulholland-drive-2016-04

    Nolan, C. (2010). Inception. Warner Bros. Pictures.

    Prince, S. (2013). Digital visual effects in cinema: The seduction of reality. Rutgers University Press.

    Sider, L., Freeman, D., & Sider, J. (2013). Soundscape and soundtrack. John Wiley & Sons.

    Young, B. (2000). Jurassic Park. Universal Pictures.

  • Character Identification Theory

    he character identification theory is a psychological theory that proposes that individuals tend to identify with fictional characters in media, such as movies, books, and video games. This theory suggests that people are more likely to identify with characters who share their values, beliefs, and experiences, and that this identification can have a significant impact on their attitudes and behaviors.

    The concept of character identification has been studied in various fields, including psychology, media studies, and literature. Research in psychology has shown that individuals who strongly identify with fictional characters are more likely to adopt the beliefs and behaviors of those characters. For example, a study by Tamborini and Stiff (2002) found that individuals who identified with the main character in a television show were more likely to adopt the same values and behaviors as that character.

    Similarly, research in media studies has demonstrated the power of character identification in shaping audience attitudes and behaviors. For instance, a study by Cohen (2001) found that audiences who identified with a media character were more likely to have positive attitudes towards the social issues addressed in the media content.

    There are several reasons why individuals might identify with fictional characters. One of the main reasons is the perceived similarity between the individual and the character. For example, if a character in a movie is going through a difficult time that the individual has also experienced, they may be more likely to identify with that character. Additionally, individuals may identify with characters who possess qualities or values that they aspire to have themselves.

    Furthermore, character identification can be influenced by a range of factors, including the individual’s personality, cultural background, and media preferences. For example, individuals who are high in empathy may be more likely to identify with characters who are going through emotional struggles, while individuals who value independence and self-sufficiency may be more likely to identify with characters who exhibit those qualities.

    Despite the potential benefits of character identification, there are also several criticisms of this theory. One of the main criticisms is that it oversimplifies the complex relationship between media consumption and real-world behavior. While character identification can certainly influence people’s attitudes and beliefs, it is just one of many factors that can shape their behavior.

    Moreover, some critics argue that character identification can have negative effects on individuals, particularly if the characters they identify with exhibit problematic behaviors or attitudes. For example, a study by Mullin and Linz (1995) found that men who identified with violent media characters were more likely to engage in aggressive behavior.

    Another potential limitation of the character identification theory is that it may not fully capture the complexity of the audience-media relationship. For example, individuals may identify with multiple characters in a single media text, or they may identify with a character in one media text but not in another.

    Despite these criticisms, the character identification theory remains an important perspective in the study of media effects. By considering the ways in which individuals identify with fictional characters, researchers and practitioners can better understand how media content influences audience attitudes and behaviors.

    In conclusion, the character identification theory provides an interesting perspective on the role of fictional characters in media, but it is important to consider its limitations and the broader context in which media consumption occurs. Future research can build upon this theory by exploring the nuances of character identification and its effects on different types of individuals and media content.

    References:

    • Cohen, J. (2001). Defining identification: A theoretical look at the identification of audiences with media characters. Mass Communication and Society, 4(3), 245-264.
    • Mullin, C. R., & Linz, D. (1995). Desensitization and resensitization to violence against women: Effects of exposure to sexually violent films on judgments of domestic violence victims. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(3), 449-459.
    • Tamborini.R, & Stiff, J. B. (2002). Exploring the role of identification in the enjoyment of mediated experiences. Journal of Communication, 52(3), 437-452.
    • Zillmann, D., & Bryant, J. (1994). Entertainment as media effect. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (pp. 437-461). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    •  Van Loon, J. (2020). Character Identification: Theoretical Model and Empirical Findings. International Journal of Communication, 14, 15.
    • Giles, D. C. (2010). Media psychology. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    •  Slater, M. D. (2007). Reinforcing spirals: The mutual influence of media selectivity and media effects and their impact on individual behavior and social identity. Communication Theory, 17(3), 281-303.
  • Framing

    Framing is a complex process that involves selectively emphasizing certain aspects of a story or issue while downplaying or omitting others to shape the audience’s perception and interpretation of the event. The concept of framing has been widely discussed in media studies and communication research, as it plays a critical role in shaping public opinion and behavior.

    Agenda-setting is one communication theory that is closely linked to framing. Agenda-setting refers to the process by which the media selects and emphasizes certain topics or issues, thereby influencing what the public considers important (McCombs & Shaw, 1972). Framing is a key part of this process, as the way a story is framed can determine whether it becomes a top priority in the news or is overlooked altogether.

    Research has shown that framing can have a powerful effect on public opinion and behavior. For example, a study by Entman (1993) found that media framing of racial protests influenced public opinion about the protests and the protesters. The study found that the media’s focus on violence and disorder in the protests led the public to view the protests as more violent and disruptive than they actually were. Similarly, a study by Iyengar (1991) found that the way the media framed the issue of crime and violence in the United States influenced public attitudes towards crime and support for tougher criminal justice policies.

    Framing has also been linked to cultivation theory. Cultivation theory suggests that media can shape people’s perceptions of reality by portraying certain messages and images repeatedly over time (Gerbner & Gross, 1976). Framing plays a key role in cultivation theory, as the way a story is framed can determine how often and in what context it is presented to the audience.

    Finally, social identity theory has also been linked to framing. Social identity theory suggests that people’s sense of self is shaped by their social group membership and the way that group is portrayed in the media (Tajfel & Turner, 1979). Framing can influence the way that social groups are portrayed in the media, and thus can influence how people identify with those groups.

    In conclusion, framing is a critical concept in media studies and communication research. It is closely linked to other communication theories, such as agenda-setting, cultivation theory, and social identity theory. Research has shown that framing can have a powerful effect on public opinion and behavior, highlighting the importance of understanding the role of framing in media and communication.

    References:

    Entman, R. M. (1993). Framing: Toward clarification of a fractured paradigm. Journal of communication, 43(4), 51-58.

    Gerbner, G., & Gross, L. (1976). Living with television: The violence profile. Journal of communication, 26(2), 173-199.

    Iyengar, S. (1991). Is anyone responsible? How television frames political issues. University of Chicago Press.

    McCombs, M. E., & Shaw, D. L. (1972). The agenda-setting function of mass media. Public opinion quarterly, 36(2), 176-187.

    Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. The social psychology of intergroup relations, 33(47), 74.

  • Ten Media Theories and their Criticism


    1. Hypodermic Needle Theory

    Hypodermic Needle Theory suggests that media messages are directly injected into the audience and have an immediate and powerful effect. Some early research supported this theory, such as the famous “War of the Worlds” broadcast in 1938 that caused widespread panic among listeners. However, subsequent research has discredited the theory, showing that media effects are more complex and subtle than the theory suggests (McQuail, 2010). For example, a meta-analysis of research on media violence and aggression found that the relationship between media exposure and aggression was weak and that other factors, such as family environment and peer influence, played a more important role (Ferguson, 2015).

    1. Cultivation Theory

    Cultivation Theory suggests that the more people are exposed to media messages, the more they are likely to adopt the values and beliefs depicted in those messages. Some research has supported this theory, such as the famous “Mean World” syndrome described by Gerbner, which suggests that heavy television viewers have a more negative and fearful view of the world. However, critics argue that the theory overemphasizes the impact of television on attitudes and behaviors, and that it does not account for the role of other factors such as social interactions and personal experiences (Shrum, 2012). Furthermore, recent research has challenged the notion that media exposure is a strong predictor of attitudes and behaviors, and has suggested that other factors, such as social identity and group norms, may play a more important role (Slater & Rouner, 2002).

    1. Agenda-Setting Theory

    Agenda-Setting Theory suggests that the media has the power to influence what people think about by deciding which issues and topics to focus on. This theory has been supported by a considerable body of research, including studies that have found a strong correlation between media coverage and public opinion (McCombs & Shaw, 1972). However, critics argue that the theory does not account for the influence of social and cultural factors on individual perceptions of the media’s agenda, and that the relationship between media coverage and public opinion is complex and mediated by other factors, such as personal values and attitudes (Weaver & Bimber, 2012).

    1. Social Learning Theory

    Social Learning Theory posits that people learn by observing the behavior of others and imitating it. This theory suggests that media can shape people’s behaviors and attitudes by providing models for imitation. Some research has supported this theory, such as studies that have found a correlation between media violence and aggression (Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1963). However, critics argue that the theory overlooks the role of cognitive processing and personal agency, and that the effects of media exposure on behavior are mediated by individual factors such as personality and context (Gentile, 2009).

    1. Uses and Gratifications Theory

    Uses and Gratifications Theory suggests that people use media to fulfill specific needs and desires, such as the need for entertainment or the desire for social interaction. This theory posits that individuals actively select and use media to meet their needs and that media consumption can be a gratifying and rewarding experience. Some research has supported this theory, such as studies that have found a correlation between media use and life satisfaction (Rubin, 2002). However, critics argue that the theory oversimplifies the complex relationship between media and human behavior, and that it does not account for the influence of social and cultural factors on media use (Katz, Blumler, & Gurevitch, 1973).

    1. Two-Step Flow Theory

    Two-Step Flow Theory suggests that media messages are first received by opinion leaders, who then transmit those messages to the wider public. This theory suggests that people are more influenced by their social networks than by the media itself. Some research has supported this theory, such as studies that have found that opinion leaders play a key role in disseminating political information (Katz & Lazarsfeld, 1955). However, critics argue that the theory does not account for the role of the media in shaping public opinion, and that it overlooks the fact that opinion leaders themselves are often influenced by media messages (McLeod, Kosicki, & McLeod, 2002).

    1. Spiral of Silence Theory

    Spiral of Silence Theory suggests that people are more likely to express their opinions when they perceive that those opinions are popular and widely accepted, and are less likely to express their opinions when they perceive that those opinions are unpopular or marginalized. This theory suggests that media can shape public opinion by creating the perception of a dominant or marginalized discourse. Some research has supported this theory, such as studies that have found that people are more likely to conform to the majority opinion when they perceive that their opinion is unpopular (Noelle-Neumann, 1984). However, critics argue that the theory does not account for the role of individual factors such as personality and motivation, and that it oversimplifies the complex relationship between media and public opinion (Mutz, 1992).

    1. Third-Person Effect Theory

    Third-Person Effect Theory suggests that people tend to overestimate the influence of media messages on other people, while underestimating the influence of those messages on themselves. This theory suggests that media can shape public opinion by creating a false perception of the impact of media messages. Some research has supported this theory, such as studies that have found that people are more likely to support censorship of media content that they perceive as having a negative influence on others (Davison, 1983). However, critics argue that the theory overlooks the role of cognitive biases and individual differences in perceptions of media effects, and that it oversimplifies the complex relationship between media and public opinion (Gunther & Storey, 2003).

    1. Technological Determinism Theory

    Technological Determinism Theory suggests that technology is the primary driver of social change and that it has a deterministic impact on human behavior and culture. This theory suggests that media can shape human behavior by providing new tools and platforms for communication and interaction. Some research has supported this theory, such as studies that have found that social media use is correlated with changes in social and political behavior (Shirky, 2011). However, critics argue that the theory overlooks the role of human agency and social factors in shaping technological development and use, and that it oversimplifies the complex relationship between technology and society (Cheney-Lippold, 2011).

    1. Cultural Studies Theory

    Cultural Studies Theory suggests that media is a key site of cultural production and that it plays a central role in shaping and reflecting cultural values and identities. This theory suggests that media can shape cultural norms and values by representing and reinforcing dominant discourses and ideologies. Some research has supported this theory, such as studies that have found that media representations of race and gender can influence social attitudes and behaviors (Van Zoonen, 2005). However, critics argue that the theory overlooks the agency and resistance of audiences in interpreting and negotiating media messages, and that it overemphasizes the power of media in shaping culture (Fiske, 1989).

    media theories provide valuable insights into the complex relationship between media and society, but they are not without limitations and criticisms. It is important to consider both supporting and counterarguments when evaluating media theories, and to recognize the complexity and diversity of media effects.

    Moreover, it is crucial to acknowledge the role of individual differences, cultural and societal contexts, and other factors that can impact the relationship between media and public opinion. As media technologies continue to evolve and reshape our society, it is essential to remain critical and informed consumers of media and to engage in ongoing discussions about the impact of media on our lives.

    References:

    Cheney-Lippold, J. (2011). A new algorithmic identity: Soft biopolitics and the modulation of control. Theory, Culture & Society, 28(6), 164–181.

    Davison, W. P. (1983). The third-person effect in communication. Public Opinion Quarterly, 47(1), 1–15.

    Fiske, J. (1989). Reading the popular. Unwin Hyman.

    Gunther, A. C., & Storey, J. D. (2003). The influence of presumed influence. Journal of Communication, 53(2), 199–215.

    Katz, E., & Lazarsfeld, P. F. (1955). Personal influence: The part played by people in the flow of mass communications. Free Press.

    McLeod, J. M., Kosicki, G. M., & McLeod, D. M. (2002). The expanding boundaries of mass media effects. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 63–90). Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Mutz, D. C. (1992). Mass media and the spiral of silence. Communication Research, 19(1), 3–35.

    Noelle-Neumann, E. (1984). The spiral of silence: A theory of public opinion. Journal of Communication, 24(2), 43–51.

    Shirky, C. (2011). The political power of social media. Foreign Affairs, 90(1), 28–41.

    Van Zoonen, L. (2005). Entertaining the citizen: When politics and popular culture converge. Rowman & Littlefield.

    Theory #9: Cultivation Theory

    Cultivation theory suggests that heavy exposure to media content, particularly television, can shape an individual’s view of the world and their beliefs about social reality. This theory proposes that the repeated exposure to media content can “cultivate” an individual’s perception of social reality and create a shared perception of social norms, values, and beliefs.

    Supporting Sources:

    1. Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., & Signorielli, N. (1980). The “mainstreaming” of America: Violence profile no. 11. Journal of Communication, 30(3), 10–29.
    2. Morgan, M., & Shanahan, J. (2010). The state of cultivation. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 54(2), 337–355.
    3. Shrum, L. J. (1996). The role of media consumption in the formation of environmental concern. The Journal of Social Issues, 52(3), 157–175.
    4. Signorielli, N. (2003). Cultivation analysis: An overview. Mass Communication & Society, 6(2), 175–194.

    Counterarguments:

    1. Critics argue that cultivation theory oversimplifies the relationship between media exposure and social reality, as it does not account for other factors that may shape an individual’s beliefs and attitudes, such as personal experiences, social interactions, and cultural values.
    2. Some studies have found that the relationship between media exposure and cultivation is not as strong as initially proposed, and that other factors such as demographic characteristics, lifestyle, and personality traits may impact the relationship between media exposure and belief systems (Shanahan & Morgan, 1999).
    3. Critics also argue that cultivation theory does not consider the diverse media landscape, where individuals have access to a broad range of media sources and can actively select and interpret media content based on their preferences and values (Giles & Maltby, 2004).
    4. Moreover, some studies have found that media effects on cultivation may vary across different cultural and societal contexts, suggesting that the theory’s applicability is limited to certain settings and populations (Shanahan & Morgan, 1999).

    References:

    Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., & Signorielli, N. (1980). The “mainstreaming” of America: Violence profile no. 11. Journal of Communication, 30(3), 10–29.

    Giles, D., & Maltby, J. (2004). The role of media figures in the social construction of celebrity: Mediated and self-mediated celebrity. Celebrity Studies, 1(3), 311–322.

    Morgan, M., & Shanahan, J. (2010). The state of cultivation. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 54(2), 337–355.

    Shanahan, J., & Morgan, M. (1999). Television and its viewers: Cultivation theory and research. Cambridge University Press.

    Shrum, L. J. (1996). The role of media consumption in the formation of environmental concern. The Journal of Social Issues, 52(3), 157–175.

    Signorielli, N. (2003). Cultivation analysis: An overview. Mass Communication & Society, 6(2), 175–194.

    Theory #10: Uses and Gratifications Theory

    Uses and gratifications theory proposes that individuals actively seek out and use media to fulfill specific needs and desires, such as information, entertainment, socialization, and identity formation. This theory suggests that individuals are not passive recipients of media messages, but rather active consumers who select and interpret media 

    content based on their motivations, preferences, and needs.

    Supporting Sources:

    1. Katz, E., Blumler, J. G., & Gurevitch, M. (1974). Utilization of mass communication by the individual. In The uses of mass communications: Current perspectives on gratifications research (pp. 19-32). Sage Publications, Inc.
    2. Ruggiero, T. E. (2000). Uses and gratifications theory in the 21st century. Mass Communication & Society, 3(1), 3–37.
    3. Rubin, A. M. (2002). The uses-and-gratifications perspective of media effects. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (pp. 525–548). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
    4. Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2013). The differential susceptibility to media effects model. Journal of Communication, 63(2), 221–243.

    Counterarguments:

    1. Critics argue that uses and gratifications theory overlooks the power of media to shape individuals’ beliefs and values, as it focuses primarily on the individual’s motivations and needs rather than the media’s influence (McQuail, 2010).
    2. Some scholars suggest that uses and gratifications theory may not fully capture the complex ways in which individuals consume media and that other factors such as social context, media content, and personal characteristics may also impact the relationship between media and individual needs (Bartsch, Vorderer, Mangold, & Reinemann, 2008).
    3. Moreover, some studies have found that the relationship between media use and individual needs may vary across different contexts and media types, suggesting that the theory’s generalizability is limited (Ruggiero, 2000).
    4. Critics also argue that uses and gratifications theory does not account for the power structures and commercial interests that shape media content and limit individuals’ choices and access to alternative media sources (Holtzman, 2000).

    References:

    Bartsch, A., Vorderer, P., Mangold, R., & Reinemann, C. (2008). Does the medium matter? The impact of new media on traditional media usage. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 52(4), 675–696.

    Holtzman, L. (2000). Media messages and socialization: Reconsidering Uses and Gratifications. In D. Zillmann, & P. Vorderer (Eds.), Media entertainment: The psychology of its appeal (pp. 51-64). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Katz, E., Blumler, J. G., & Gurevitch, M. (1974). Utilization of mass communication by the individual. In The uses of mass communications: Current perspectives on gratifications research (pp. 19-32). Sage Publications, Inc.

    McQuail, D. (2010). McQuail’s mass communication theory. Sage.

    Rubin, A. M. (2002). The uses-and-gratifications perspective of media effects. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (pp. 525–548). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Ruggiero, T. E. (2000). Uses and gratifications theory in the 21st century. Mass Communication & Society, 3(1), 3–37.

    Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2013). The differential susceptibility to media effects model. Journal of Communication, 63(2), 221–243.

    1. Agenda Setting Theory

    Agenda setting theory suggests that the media has a powerful influence on the public by setting the agenda for what issues are important and how they should be understood. According to this theory, the media’s selection and emphasis on certain news topics and frames have a significant impact on public perception and priorities. This theory was first introduced by McCombs and Shaw in 1972, based on the results of a study that found a strong correlation between the media’s coverage of specific issues and their perceived importance by the public.

    Supporting Sources:

    1. McCombs, M., & Shaw, D. (1972). The agenda-setting function of mass media. Public Opinion Quarterly, 36(2), 176–187.
    2. McCombs, M. E., & Reynolds, A. (2009). How the news shapes our civic agenda. In A. Chadwick & P. N. Howard (Eds.), The Routledge Handbook of Internet Politics (pp. 227–238). Routledge.
    3. Scheufele, D. A., & Tewksbury, D. (2007). Framing, agenda setting, and priming: The evolution of three media effects models. Journal of Communication, 57(1), 9–20.
    4. Shapiro, I. (2013). The evolution of agenda-setting research: Twenty-five years in the marketplace of ideas. Journal of Communication, 63(4), 96–103.

    Counterarguments:

    1. Critics of agenda setting theory argue that it oversimplifies the complex relationship between the media and the public by ignoring the role of other factors, such as interpersonal communication, in shaping public opinion (Mutz, 1992).
    2. Some scholars suggest that agenda setting theory fails to account for the power dynamics between the media and political elites, who may use the media to set the agenda in their favor and limit the scope of public debate (Entman, 2004).
    3. Moreover, research has shown that the relationship between media coverage and public opinion is more complex than just a one-way influence, and that the public may also influence the media agenda (McLeod, Kosicki, & McLeod, 1994).
    4. Other scholars have criticized agenda setting theory for being too narrow in scope, focusing primarily on political and policy issues and neglecting the role of the media in shaping public attitudes and behaviors related to other topics such as entertainment, lifestyle, and health (Zhu, Sherry, Chen, & Lu, 2018).

    References:

    Entman, R. M. (2004). Projections of power: Framing news, public opinion, and US foreign policy. University of Chicago Press.

    McCombs, M., & Shaw, D. (1972). The agenda-setting function of mass media. Public Opinion Quarterly, 36(2), 176–187.

    McLeod, J. M., Kosicki, G. M., & McLeod, D. M. (1994). The expanding boundaries of agenda-setting: From the mass media to the public agenda. In J. A. Anderson (Ed.), Communication Yearbook 17 (pp. 48–67). Sage.

    Mutz, D. C. (1992). Mass media and the concept of interdependence: The case of the Gulf War. Political Communication, 9(1), 47–64.

    Scheufele, D. A., & Tewksbury, D. (2007). Framing, agenda setting, and priming: The evolution of three media effects models. Journal of Communication, 57(1), 9–20.

    Shapiro, I. (2013). The evolution of agenda

    1. Cultivation Theory

    Cultivation theory suggests that long-term exposure to media content, particularly on television, can shape individuals’ perceptions of reality and social norms. According to this theory, people who watch a lot of television are more likely to view the world in ways that align with the media’s portrayal of social life. This theory was first introduced by George Gerbner in the 1960s and has been influential in shaping research on media effects.

    Supporting Sources:

    1. Gerbner, G. (1969). Toward “cultural indicators”: The analysis of mass mediated public message systems. AV Communication Review, 17(2), 137–148.
    2. Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., & Signorielli, N. (1994). Growing up with television: The cultivation perspective. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media Effects: Advances in Theory and Research (pp. 17–41). Routledge.
    3. Morgan, M., & Shanahan, J. (2010). The state of cultivation. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 54(2), 337–355.
    4. Shrum, L. J., Wyer, R. S. Jr., & O’Guinn, T. C. (1998). The effects of television consumption on social perceptions: The use of priming procedures to investigate psychological processes. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(4), 447–458.

    Counterarguments:

    1. Critics of cultivation theory argue that it overestimates the power of media exposure and underestimates the role of other factors, such as personal experiences and social interactions, in shaping individuals’ attitudes and beliefs (Shanahan & Morgan, 1999).
    2. Some scholars suggest that the effects of media exposure may vary across different types of content, with news programming having a different impact than entertainment programming (Gross & Aday, 2003).
    3. Moreover, research has shown that individuals’ level of media literacy and critical thinking skills can mitigate the effects of media exposure (Livingstone & Helsper, 2006).
    4. Other scholars have criticized cultivation theory for being too simplistic and not accounting for the complex ways in which individuals interpret and respond to media messages (Corner, Richardson, Fenton, & Phillips, 1990).

    References:

    Corner, J., Richardson, K., Fenton, N., & Phillips, L. (1990). The art of record keeping: Cultivation analysis and contemporary television. Media, Culture & Society, 12(1), 89–102.

    Gerbner, G. (1969). Toward “cultural indicators”: The analysis of mass mediated public message systems. AV Communication Review, 17(2), 137–148.

    Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., & Signorielli, N. (1994). Growing up with television: The cultivation perspective. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media Effects: Advances in Theory and Research (pp. 17–41). Routledge.

    Gross, K. E., & Aday, S. (2003). The scary world in your living room and neighborhood: Using local broadcast news, neighborhood crime rates, and personal experience to test cultivation. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 47(3), 289–310.

    Livingstone, S., & Helsper, E. J. (2006). Does advertising literacy mediate the effects of advertising on children? A critical examination of two linked research literatures in relation to obesity and food choice. Journal of Communication, 56(3), 560

    1. Agenda Setting Theory

    Agenda setting theory suggests that the media has the power to influence what issues and topics are considered important by the public. According to this theory, the media sets the agenda by deciding what stories to cover and how to cover them, which in turn influences public opinion and political decisions. The theory was first introduced by Maxwell McCombs and Donald Shaw in 1972 and has since been widely studied in the field of media effects.

    Supporting Sources:

    1. McCombs, M., & Shaw, D. (1972). The agenda-setting function of mass media. Public Opinion Quarterly, 36(2), 176–187.
    2. Iyengar, S., & Kinder, D. R. (1987). News that matters: Television and American opinion. University of Chicago Press.
    3. Scheufele, D. A. (1999). Framing as a theory of media effects. Journal of Communication, 49(1), 103–122.
    4. Price, V., Tewksbury, D., & Powers, E. (1997). Switching trains of thought: The impact of news frames on readers’ cognitive responses. Communication Research, 24(5), 481–506.

    Counterarguments:

    1. Some critics argue that agenda setting theory overestimates the media’s influence on public opinion and neglects the role of other factors, such as personal values and beliefs, in shaping individuals’ attitudes (Iyengar & Kinder, 1987).
    2. Additionally, research has shown that the media may have a limited effect on changing public opinion, as individuals tend to seek out information that confirms their existing beliefs and attitudes (Zaller, 1992).
    3. Critics also suggest that agenda setting theory is too focused on the content of media messages and neglects the role of other factors, such as the media’s ownership and control, in shaping what issues and topics are covered (Chomsky, 1997).
    4. Some scholars have also criticized the theory for being too simplistic and not accounting for the complex ways in which individuals interpret and respond to media messages (Entman, 1993).

    References:

    Chomsky, N. (1997). What makes mainstream media mainstream. Z Magazine, 10(9), 36–41.

    Entman, R. M. (1993). Framing: Toward clarification of a fractured paradigm. Journal of Communication, 43(4), 51–58.

    Iyengar, S., & Kinder, D. R. (1987). News that matters: Television and American opinion. University of Chicago Press.

    McCombs, M., & Shaw, D. (1972). The agenda-setting function of mass media. Public Opinion Quarterly, 36(2), 176–187.

    Price, V., Tewksbury, D., & Powers, E. (1997). Switching trains of thought: The impact of news frames on readers’ cognitive responses. Communication Research, 24(5), 481–506.

    Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge University Press.

    1. Uses and Gratifications Theory

    Uses and gratifications theory suggests that individuals are active agents in their media consumption and seek out media content that satisfies their individual needs and desires. According to this theory, people use media for a variety of reasons, including entertainment, information, social interaction, and personal identity. The theory was first introduced by Elihu Katz and Jay Blumler in the 1970s and has been influential in shaping research on media consumption.

    Supporting Sources:

    1. Katz, E., Blumler, J. G., & Gure vitch, M. (1974). Uses and gratifications research. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 37(4), 509-523. 2. Ruggiero, T. E. (2000). Uses and gratifications theory in the 21st century. Mass Communication & Society, 3(1), 3-37.
      1. Papacharissi, Z. (2010). A networked self: Identity, community, and culture on social network sites. Routledge.
      2. Rubin, A. M. (1994). Media uses and effects: A uses-and-gratifications perspective. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (pp. 417-436). Routledge.
      Counterarguments:
      1. Critics argue that uses and gratifications theory oversimplifies the complex relationship between individuals and media consumption and neglects the role of media producers in shaping content to meet audience needs (Bruns, 2007).
      2. Additionally, the theory has been criticized for neglecting the role of social and cultural factors in shaping media consumption patterns, such as age, gender, and socioeconomic status (Livingstone, 2004).
      3. Some scholars have also suggested that the theory is too focused on individual motivations for media use and neglects the social and political implications of media consumption (Couldry, 2004).
      4. Others have criticized the theory for failing to account for the role of media technologies in shaping media use and gratifications, as new technologies may create new needs and desires that were not previously recognized (Papacharissi, 2010).
      References:Bruns, A. (2007). Produsage: Towards a broader framework for user-led content creation. In Proceedings Creativity & Cognition (pp. 99-106). ACM.Couldry, N. (2004). Theorising media as practice. Social Semiotics, 14(2), 115-132.Livingstone, S. (2004). Media literacy and the challenge of new information and communication technologies. The Communication Review, 7(1), 3-14.Papacharissi, Z. (2010). A networked self: Identity, community, and culture on social network sites. Routledge.Rubin, A. M. (1994). Media uses and effects: A uses-and-gratifications perspective. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (pp. 417-436). Routledge.Katz, E., Blumler, J. G., & Gurevitch, M. (1974). Uses and gratifications research. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 37(4), 509-523.
      1. Cultivation Theory
      Cultivation theory suggests that exposure to media content, particularly television, can shape individuals’ perceptions of the world and influence their attitudes and beliefs. According to this theory, individuals who consume a lot of television content are more likely to adopt the values and beliefs portrayed in that content. The theory was first introduced by George Gerbner in the 1970s and has been influential in shaping research on the effects of media on audiences.Supporting Sources:
      1. Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., & Signorielli, N. (1980). The “mainstreaming” of America: Violence profile no. 11. Journal of Communication, 30(3), 10-29.
      2. Morgan, M., & Shanahan, J. (2010). The state of cultivation. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 54(2-), 337-355. 3. Shanahan, J., & Morgan, M. (1999). Television and its viewers: Cultivation theory and research. Cambridge University Press.
        1. Shrum, L. J., Wyer, R. S., & O’Guinn, T. C. (1998). The effects of television consumption on social perceptions: The use of priming procedures to investigate psychological processes. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(4), 447-458.
        Counterarguments:
        1. Critics of cultivation theory argue that the theory overemphasizes the effects of media content on individuals’ attitudes and beliefs and neglects the role of other social and cultural factors in shaping these outcomes (Giles, 2003).
        2. Additionally, some scholars have argued that the theory is too focused on the effects of television and neglects the role of other media, such as the internet and social media, in shaping individuals’ perceptions of the world (Livingstone, 2009).
        3. Others have criticized the theory for being too simplistic in its view of media content as having a direct, one-way effect on individuals, without accounting for the complexity of the social and cultural contexts in which media consumption takes place (Ang, 1996).
        4. Finally, some have argued that the theory is not well-suited to account for the individual differences in how audiences consume and interpret media content, as different people may have different levels of media literacy and different cultural backgrounds that shape their interpretations (Gasher, 2012).
        References:Ang, I. (1996). Living room wars: Rethinking media audiences for a postmodern world. Routledge.Gasher, M. (2012). Cultivation theory. In W. Donsbach (Ed.), The International Encyclopedia of Communication (pp. 1073-1075). John Wiley & Sons.Giles, D. C. (2003). Media psychology. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Livingstone, S. (2009). On the mediation of everything: ICA Presidential Address 2008. Journal of Communication, 59(1), 1-18.Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., & Signorielli, N. (1980). The “mainstreaming” of America: Violence profile no. 11. Journal of Communication, 30(3), 10-29.Morgan, M., & Shanahan, J. (2010). The state of cultivation. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 54(2), 337-355.Shanahan, J., & Morgan, M. (1999). Television and its viewers: Cultivation theory and research. Cambridge University Press.Shrum, L. J., Wyer, R. S., & O’Guinn, T. C. (1998). The effects of television consumption on social perceptions: The use of priming procedures to investigate psychological processes. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(4), 447-458.In conclusion, media theories have contributed greatly to our understanding of the complex relationship between media and society. However, each theory has its strengths and limitations, and it is important to consider counterarguments and alternative perspectives in order to develop a more nuanced and complete understanding of media effects. By critically evaluating these theories and engaging with a range of perspectives, we can develop a more comprehensive understanding of how media shapes our lives and society as a whole
  • Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS)

    Mindfulness has become an increasingly popular concept in recent years, as people strive to find ways to reduce stress, increase focus, and improve their overall wellbeing. One of the most widely used tools for measuring mindfulness is the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS), developed by J. Brown and R. Ryan in 2003. In this blog post, we will explore the MAAS and its different scales to help you better understand how it can be used to measure mindfulness.

    The MAAS is a 15-item scale designed to measure the extent to which individuals are able to maintain a non-judgmental and present-focused attention to their thoughts and sensations in daily life. The scale consists of statements that are rated on a six-point scale ranging from 1 (almost always) to 6 (almost never). Respondents are asked to indicate how frequently they have experienced each statement over the past week.

    The MAAS is divided into three subscales, which can be used to measure different aspects of mindfulness. The first subscale is the Attention subscale, which measures the extent to which individuals are able to maintain their focus on the present moment. The second subscale is the Awareness subscale, which measures the extent to which individuals are able to notice their thoughts and sensations without judging them. The third subscale is the Acceptance subscale, which measures the extent to which individuals are able to accept their thoughts and feelings without trying to change them.

    Each subscale of the MAAS consists of five items. Here are the items included in each subscale:

    Attention Subscale:

    1. I find myself doing things without paying attention.
    2. I drive places on “automatic pilot” and then wonder why I went there.
    3. I find myself easily distracted during tasks.
    4. I tend not to notice feelings of physical tension or discomfort until they really grab my attention.
    5. I rush through activities without being really attentive to them.

    Awareness Subscale:

    1. I could be experiencing some emotion and not be conscious of it until sometime later.
    2. I break or spill things because of carelessness, not paying attention, or thinking of something else.
    3. I find it difficult to stay focused on what’s happening in the present.
    4. I find myself preoccupied with the future or the past.
    5. I find myself listening to someone with one ear, doing something else at the same time.

    Acceptance Subscale:

    1. I tell myself that I shouldn’t be feeling the way that I’m feeling.
    2. When I fail at something important to me I become consumed by feelings of inadequacy.
    3. When I’m feeling down I tend to obsess and fixate on everything

    Awareness Subscale:

    1. I could be experiencing some emotion and not be conscious of it until sometime later.
    2. I break or spill things because of carelessness, not paying attention, or thinking of something else.
    3. I find it difficult to stay focused on what’s happening in the present.
    4. I find myself preoccupied with the future or the past.
    5. I find myself listening to someone with one ear, doing something else at the same time.

    Acceptance Subscale:

    1. I tell myself that I shouldn’t be feeling the way that I’m feeling.
    2. When I fail at something important to me I become consumed by feelings of inadequacy.
    3. When I’m feeling down I tend to obsess and fixate on everything
  • Concepts and Variables

    Concepts and variables are two key terms that play a significant role in media studies. While the two terms may appear similar, they serve distinct purposes and meanings. Understanding the differences between concepts and variables is essential for media studies scholars and students. In this blog post, we will explore the distinctions between concepts and variables in the context of media studies. 

    Concepts: 

    Concepts are abstract ideas that help to classify and describe phenomena. They are essential in media studies as they help in creating an understanding of the objects of study. Concepts are used to develop mental models of media objects, to analyze and critique them. For example, concepts such as “representation” and “power” are used to describe and understand how media texts work (Kellner, 2015). 

    Variables: 

    Variables, on the other hand, are used to store data in a program or research. They are crucial in media studies research as they help in collecting and analyzing data. Variables are named containers that hold a specific value, such as numerical or textual data. Variables can be manipulated and changed during the research process. For example, variables such as age, gender, and socio-economic status can be used to collect data and analyze the relationship between media and society (Morgan & Shanahan, 2010). 

    Differences: 

    One of the significant differences between concepts and variables is that concepts are abstract while variables are concrete. Concepts are used to create mental models that help to understand and analyze media objects, while variables are used to collect and analyze data in research. Another difference is that concepts are broader and at a higher level than variables. Concepts are used to describe the overall structure and design of media texts, while variables are used to study specific aspects of media objects. 

    In addition, concepts are often used to group together related variables in media studies research. For example, the concept of “media effects” might be used to group variables such as exposure to media, attitude change, and behavior change. By grouping related variables together, researchers can have a better understanding of the complex relationships between variables and concepts in media studies research. 

    Concepts and Variables are two essential components of media studies research. Concepts help to develop mental models of media objects, while variables are used to collect and analyze data in research. By understanding the differences between these two terms, media studies scholars and students can create more effective and efficient research.

  • Theories, Models and Concepts

    Theories, Models, and Concepts in Media and Marketing

    In the realm of media and marketing, understanding theories, models, and concepts is crucial for developing effective strategies. These constructs provide a framework for analyzing consumer behavior, crafting strategies, and implementing marketing campaigns. This essay will explore each construct with examples to illustrate their application.

    Theories

    Definition: Theories in marketing and media are systematic explanations of phenomena that predict how certain variables interact. They help marketers understand consumer behavior and the effectiveness of different strategies.

    Example: Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs

    • Theory: Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs is a psychological theory that suggests human actions are motivated by a progression of needs, from basic physiological requirements to self-actualization[3].
    • Model: In marketing, this theory is modeled by identifying which level of need a product or service satisfies. For example, a luxury car brand might focus on self-esteem needs by promoting exclusivity and status.
    • Concept: The concept derived from this model is “status marketing,” where products are marketed as symbols of success and achievement to appeal to consumers seeking self-esteem fulfillment.

    Models

    Definition: Models are simplified representations of reality that help marketers visualize complex processes and make predictions. They often serve as tools for strategic planning.

    Example: AIDA Model

    • Theory: The AIDA model is based on the theory that consumers go through four stages before making a purchase: Attention, Interest, Desire, and Action[2].
    • Model: This model guides marketers in structuring their advertising campaigns to first capture attention with striking visuals or headlines, then build interest with engaging content, create desire by highlighting benefits, and finally prompt action with clear calls to action.
    • Concept: The concept here is “customer journey mapping,” where marketers design each stage of interaction to lead the consumer smoothly from awareness to purchase.

    Concepts

    Definition: Concepts are ideas or mental constructs that arise from theories and models. They provide actionable insights or strategies for marketers.

    Example: Content Marketing

    • Theory: Content marketing is grounded in the theory that providing valuable content builds brand awareness and trust among consumers[2].
    • Model: A content marketing model involves creating a mix of informative blogs, engaging videos, and interactive social media posts to attract and retain an audience.
    • Concept: The concept derived from this model is “brand storytelling,” where brands use narratives to connect emotionally with their audience, fostering loyalty and engagement.

    In the realm of media and marketing, understanding theories, models, and concepts is crucial for developing effective strategies. These constructs provide a framework for analyzing consumer behavior, crafting strategies, and implementing marketing campaigns. This essay will explore each construct with examples to illustrate their application.